Uma revisão das definições de “esforço” nos artigos publicados no Journal of the Experimental Behavior Analysis (JEAB) e Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (JABA)
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.33448/rsd-v13i5.45975Palavras-chave:
Esforço físico, Comportamento, Revisão sistemática.Resumo
Por meio de uma revisão da literatura, este trabalho mapeou as concepções analítico comportamentais de “esforço” apresentadas nos dois primeiros periódicos especializados em análise do comportamento: JEAB e JABA. O objetivo do presente estudo foi identificar, organizar e apresentar diferentes conceituações da definição de esforço nas publicações analítico-comportamentais, bem como categorizar as manipulações experimentais utilizadas para investigar essa variável em estudos publicados no JEAB e no JABA. Foram selecionados artigos que delimitavam “esforço” como uma das variáveis experimentais. Foram encontradas quatro categorias de definições de “esforço”: (1) esforço como exigências do esquema de reforço; (2) esforço como exigência física requerida para cada resposta; (3) esforço como requisitos de distância ou altura; (4) esforço como complexidade geral da tarefa. Observou-se maior preponderância de manipulações da Categoria 2 nos artigos do JEAB, e da Categoria 3 no JABA. A despeito da variedade nas definições de esforço, observa-se que (a) é um conceito que descreve uma característica intrínseca da resposta se considerarmos que qualquer ação exige algum nível de esforço e que (b) sua definição varia de acordo com o arranjo de contingências no qual o organismo se comporta.
Downloads
Referências
Adair, E. R., & Wright, B. A. (1976). Behavioral thermoregulation in the squirrel monkey when response effort is varied. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 90(2), 179–184. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077197
Aiken, E. G. (1957). The effort variable in the acquisition, extinction, and spontaneous recovery of an instrumental response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 47–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040161
Alessandri, J., & Lattal, K. A. (2021). Early‐extinction effects on a force response of humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 115(3), 667-678. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.683
Alling, K., & Poling, A. (1995). The effects of differing response-force requirements on fixed-ratio responding of rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 63(3), 331–346. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1995.63-331
Aparicio, C. F. (2001). Overmatching in Rats: the Barrier Choice Paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75(1), 93–106. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2001.75-93
Baum, W. M. (1979). Matching, undermatching, and overmatching in studies of choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 32 (2), 269-281. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1332902/pdf/jea¬behav00086-0135.pdf
Brackney, R. J., Cheung, T. H., Neisewander, J. L., & Sanabria, F. (2011). The isolation of motivational, motoric, and schedule effects on operant performance: A modeling approach. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96(1), 17-38. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.96-17
Case, D. A., Fantino, E., & Wixted, J. (1985). Human observing: maintained by negative informative stimuli only if correlated with improvement in response efficiency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43(3), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1985.43-289
Casella, S. E., Wilder, D. A., Neidert, P., Rey, C., Compton, M., & Chong, I. (2010). The effects of response effort on safe performance by therapists at an autism treatment facility. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 729-734. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-729
Chung, S.-H. (1965). Effects of effort on response rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1965.8-1
Clement, T. S., Feltus, J. R., Kaiser, D. H., & Zentall, T. R. (2000). “Work ethic” in pigeons: reward value is directly related to the effort or time required to obtain the reward. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7(1), 100–106. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210727
Courtney, K., & Perone, M. (1992). Reductions in Shock Frequency and Response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3(3), 485–496.
Eisenberger, R., Mitchell, M., McDermitt, M., & Masterson, F. A. (1984). Accuracy Versus Speed in the Generalized Effort of Learning-Disabled Children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42(1), 19–36. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1984.42-19
Elsmore, T. F. (1971). Effects of response effort on discrimination performance. The Psychological Record, 21(1), 17–24.https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1971-30465-001
Friman, P. C., & Poling, A. (1995). Making life easier with effort: Basic findings and applied research on response effort. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(4), 583–590. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-583
Fritz, J. N., Dupuis, D. L., Wu, W. L., Neal, A. E., Rettig, L. A., & Lastrapes, R. E. (2017). Evaluating increased effort for item disposal to improve recycling at a university. Journal of applied behavior analysis, 50(4), 825-829. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.405
Fortes, I., Vasconcelos, M., & Machado, A. (2015). The effect of response rate on reward value in a self-control task. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 103, 141–152. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.123
García‐Leal, Ó., Barrón, E., Camarena‐Pérez, H., & Vílchez, Z. (2019). Response rate correlates with indifference points in a delay‐discounting procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 112(2), 167-176. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.548
Gomes, I. S., & Caminha, I. O. (2014). Guia para estudos de revisão sistemática: uma opção metodológica para as Ciências do Movimento Humano. Movimento, 20(1), 395-411.
Hoffman, S. H., Branch, M. N., & Sizemore, G. M. (1987). Cocaine tolerance: Acute versus chronic effects as dependent upon fixed‐ratio size. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47(3), 363-376. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1987.47-363
Herrick, R. M. (1964). The Successive Differentiation of a Lever Displacement Response. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7(3), 211–215. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1964.7-211
Horner, R. H., & Day, H. M. (1991). The effects of response efficiency on functionally equivalent competing behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24(4), 719–732. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1991.24-719
Hunter, I., & Davison, M. (1982). Independence of response force and reinforcement rate on concurrent variable‐interval schedule performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37(2), 183-197. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1982.37-183
Irvin, D. S., Thompson, T. J., Turner, W. D., & Williams, D. E. (1998). Utilizing increased response effort to reduce chronic hand mouthing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(3), 375-385. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-375
Johnson, D. F., & Collier, G. (1991). The relationship between feeding rate and patch choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(1), 79-95. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1991.55-79
Lozy, E. D., & Sy, J. R. (2019). Evaluating different values of effort and reinforcement parameters under concurrent‐and single‐operant arrangements. Journal of applied behavior analysis, 52(2), 516-533. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.533
Mattos, P. C. (2015). Tipos de revisão de literatura. Unesp, 1-9. Recuperado de https://www.fca.unesp.br/Home/Biblioteca/tipos-de-evisao-de-literatura.pdf
McClure, G. Y. H., Hardwick, W. C., & McMillan, D. E. (2000). Effect of drugs on response‐duration differentiation VII: Response‐force requirements. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74(3), 295-309. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2000.74-295
Mcdowell, J. J., & Wood, H. M. (1985). Confirmation of linear system theory prediction: rate of change of Herrnstein’s k as a function of response-force requirement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43(1), 61–73. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1985.43-61
Miller, L. K. (1968a). Escape From an Effortful Situation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11(5), 619–627. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1968.11-619
Miller, L. K. (1968b). The effect of response force on avoidance rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11(6), 809–812. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1968.11-809
Miller, L. K. (1970). Some punishing effects of response‐force. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13(2), 215-220. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1970.13-215
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLOS Medicine, 6(7), e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed1000097
Navarro, A. D., & Fantino, E. (2005). The sunk cost effect in pigeons and humans. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 83(1), 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2005.21-04
Neef, N. A., Shade, D., & Miller, M. S. (1994). Assessing influential dimensions of reinforcers on choice in students with serious emotional disturbance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(4), 575–583.https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-575
Perone, M., & Baron, A. (1980). Reinforcement of Human Observing Behavior By a Stimulus Correlated With Extinction or Increased Effort. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34(3), 239–261. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1980.34-239
Perry, A. C., & Fisher, W. W. (2001). Behavioral economic influences on treatments designed to decrease destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(2), 211–215. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-211
Piazza, C. C., Roane, H. S., Keeney, K. M., Boney, B. R., & Abt, K. A. (2002). Varying response effort in the treatment of pica maintained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(3), 233–246. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-233
Piazza, C. C., Roane, H. S., Keeney, K. M., Boney, B. R., & Abt, K. A. (2002). Varying response effort in the treatment of pica maintained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(3), 233-246. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-233
Pinkston, J. W., & Foss, E. K. (2018). The role of response force on the persistence and structure of behavior during extinction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 109(1), 194–209. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.306
Pinkston, J. W., & Libman, B. M. (2017). Aversive functions of response effort: Fact or artifact? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 108(1), 73–96. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.264
Pinkston, J. W., & Moore, A. (2020). The effects of force requirements on microstructure: Subcriterion responses and the mismeasurement of bouts. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.613
Polick, A. S. (2008). A systematic evaluation of variables underlying response effort manipulations [Auburn University]. http://etd.auburn.edu/handle/10415/1501
Richman, D. M., Wacker, D. P., & Winborn, L. (2001). Response efficiency during functional communication training: Effects of effort on response allocation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(1), 73-76. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-73
Rilling, M., Kramer, T. J., & Askew, H. R. (1970). A preliminary analysis of the dynamics of the pecking response in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13(2), 267-278. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1970.13-267
Schroeder, S. R. (1972). Parametric effects of reinforcement frequency, amount of reinforcement, and required response force on sheltered workshop behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5(4), 431-441. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1972.5-431
Skinner, B. F. (2005). Science and Human Behavior. In B. F. Skinner Foundation. B. F. Skinner Foundation. http://www.bfskinner.org/books4sale.asp
Soares, P. G., Costa, C. E., Aló, R. M., Luiz, A., & Cunha, T. R. de L. (2017). Custo da resposta: Como tem sido definido e estudado? Perspectivas Em Análise Do Comportamento, 8(2), 258–268. https://doi.org/10.18761/PAC.2017.020
Spetch, M. L., & Treit, D. (1986). Does effort play a role in the effect of response requirements on delayed matching to sample? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45(1), 19-31. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1986.45-19
Sumpter, C. E., Temple, W., & Foster, T. M. (1998). Response form, force, and number: Effects on concurrent‐schedule performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 70(1), 45-68. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1998.70-45
Todorov, J. C., & Henriques, M. B. (2013). O que não é e o que pode vir a ser comportamento. Revista Brasileira de Análise Do Comportamento, 9(1), 74–78. https://doi.org/10.18542/rebac.v9i1.2133
Van Camp, C. M., Vollmer, T. R., & Daniel, D. (2001). A systematic evaluation of stimulus preference, response effort, and stimulus control in the treatment of automatically reinforced self-injury. Behavior Therapy, 32(3), 603–613. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80037-X
Vasconcelos, M., & Urcuioli, P. J. (2009). Extensive Training Is Insufficient To Produce the Work-Ethic Effect in Pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91(1), 143–152. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2009.91-143
Vasconcelos M, Urcuioli PJ, Lionello-DeNolf KM. Failure to replicate the 'work ethic" effect in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav. 2007 May;87(3):383-99. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2007.68-06.
Vasconcelos M, Urcuioli PJ. Extensive training is insufficient to produce the work-ethic effect in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav. 2009 Jan;91(1):143-52. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2009.91-143
Weiner, H. (1962). Some Effects of Response Cost Upon Human Operant Behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5(2), 201–208. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1962.5-201
Zarcone, T. J., Chen, R., & Fowler, S. C. (2007). Effects of differing response‐force requirements on food‐maintained responding in CD‐1 mice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88(3), 381-393. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.88-381
Zarcone, T. J., Chen, R., & Fowler, S. C. (2009). Effects of differing response‐force requirements on food‐maintained responding in C57Bl/6J mice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 92(2), 257-274. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2009.92-257
Zhou, L., Goff, G. A., & Iwata, B. A. (2000). Effects of increased response effort on self‐injury and object manipulation as competing responses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(1), 29-40. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-29
Downloads
Publicado
Edição
Seção
Licença
Copyright (c) 2024 Luan Mendes Teixeira; Myenne Mieko Ayres Tsutsumi; Thays da Cruz Silva; André Connor de Méo Luiz; Julia Röcker dos Santos

Este trabalho está licenciado sob uma licença Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Autores que publicam nesta revista concordam com os seguintes termos:
1) Autores mantém os direitos autorais e concedem à revista o direito de primeira publicação, com o trabalho simultaneamente licenciado sob a Licença Creative Commons Attribution que permite o compartilhamento do trabalho com reconhecimento da autoria e publicação inicial nesta revista.
2) Autores têm autorização para assumir contratos adicionais separadamente, para distribuição não-exclusiva da versão do trabalho publicada nesta revista (ex.: publicar em repositório institucional ou como capítulo de livro), com reconhecimento de autoria e publicação inicial nesta revista.
3) Autores têm permissão e são estimulados a publicar e distribuir seu trabalho online (ex.: em repositórios institucionais ou na sua página pessoal) a qualquer ponto antes ou durante o processo editorial, já que isso pode gerar alterações produtivas, bem como aumentar o impacto e a citação do trabalho publicado.